The University of Southern Mississippi The University of Southern Mississippi
The Aquila Digital Community The Aquila Digital Community
Honors Theses Honors College
Spring 5-2017
Jealousy and Romantic Relational Aggression Among Dating Jealousy and Romantic Relational Aggression Among Dating
College Students College Students
Ashlee A. Bryant
University of Southern Mississippi
Follow this and additional works at: https://aquila.usm.edu/honors_theses
Part of the Psychology Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Bryant, Ashlee A., "Jealousy and Romantic Relational Aggression Among Dating College Students" (2017).
Honors Theses
. 524.
https://aquila.usm.edu/honors_theses/524
This Honors College Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Honors College at The Aquila Digital
Community. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of The Aquila
Digital Community. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected].
The University of Southern Mississippi
Jealousy and Romantic Relational Aggression Among Dating College Students
by
Ashlee Bryant
A Thesis
Submitted to the Honors College of
The University of Southern Mississippi
in Partial Fulfillment
of the Requirements for the Degree of
Bachelor of Science
in the Department of Psychology
December 2016
ii
iii
Approved by
____________________________________
Eric R. Dahlen, Ph.D., Thesis Adviser
Department of Psychology
____________________________________
D. Joe Olmi, Ph.D.,
Chair
Department of Psychology
____________________________________
Ellen Weinauer, Ph.D., Dean
Honors College
iv
Abstract
Most of the research on relational aggression has been conducted with samples of older
children and early adolescents and has focused primarily on same-sex peer relationships
(Goldstein & Tisak, 2004). The aim of this study was to contribute to the relatively
meager research on relational aggression in the context of college students’ romantic
relationships by exploring the role of interpersonal jealousy. Participants included 377
undergraduate student volunteers (64 men and 313 women) ranging in age from 18 to 58
who were recruited through the Department of Psychology’s subject pool (i.e., Sona).
The data were collected in the form of an online survey hosted through the online
research system used by the Department of Psychology (i.e., Qualtrics). Measures of key
variables included the Multidimensional Jealousy Scale (MJS) and the Romantic
Relational Aggression subscale from the Self-Report of Aggression and Social Behavior
Measure (SRASBM). The scores on all three subscales of the MJS (i.e., cognitive,
emotional, and behavioral) were positively related to scores on the Romantic Relational
Aggression subscale of the SRASBM. Although all three subscales of the MJS predicted
romantic relational aggression, the Cognitive and Behavioral subscales explained the
most unique variance. The implications of these findings and the study’s limitations are
discussed.
Keywords: romantic relational aggression, jealousy, college students
v
Table of Contents
Chapter 1: Introduction…………………………………………………………………....1
Chapter 2: Literature Review……………………………………………………………...2
Relational Aggression……………………………………………………………..2
Jealousy……………………………………………………………………………4
The Present Study…………………………………………………………………6
Chapter 3: Methods……………………………………………………………………….7
Participants………………………………………………………………………..7
Measures………………………………………………………………………......8
Procedure……………………………………………………………………….....9
Chapter 4: Results……………………………………………………………..…………10
Data Screening……………………………………………………………….......10
Preliminary Analyses………………………………………………………….....11
Primary Analyses………………………………………………………………...12
Chapter 5: Discussion……………………………………………………………………13
Limitations……………………………………………………………………….15
Future Directions………………………………………………………………...16
References……………………………………………………………………………….18
Tables……………………………………………………………………………………21
IRB Approval Letter…………………………………………………………………….26
1
Introduction
Relational aggression is a form of aggressive behavior that involves the
manipulation of social relationships, as in rumor spreading or social ostracism (Goldstein,
Teran, & McFaul, 2008). Others have termed relational aggression as a set of behaviors
intended to damage another’s relationships, reputation, or feelings of belonging or
inclusion, or to exert social control (Werner & Crick, 1999). In the context of intimate
relationships, relational aggression could also involve intentionally making a romantic
partner jealous or threatening to end a relationship (Ellis, Crooks, & Wolfe, 2009).
Most of the research on relational aggression has been conducted with samples of
older children and early adolescents, and has focused primarily on same-sex peer
relationships (Goldstein & Tisak, 2004). While less is known about relational aggression
among older adolescents and emerging adults, this topic is beginning to receive increased
attention. Not only does relational aggression continue to be a significant problem for
older adolescents and early adults, but there is reason to believe that the increased
importance of intimate partnerships during this developmental period may make
relational aggression in dating relationships even more salient (Prather, Dahlen,
Nicholson, & Yowell, 2012). Linder, Crick, and Collins (2002) found that relationally
aggressive behaviors in college students’ dating relationships were associated with a
number of negative relationship qualities. Additionally, victims of romantic relational
aggression generally reported insecure dating relationships, while perpetrators reported
distrust and jealousy in their relationships (Ellis et al., 2009). Moreover, relational
aggression may be an important but often neglected aspect of the broader problem of
intimate partner violence and abuse that is unfortunately common among both teens and
2
emerging adults (Ellis et al., 2009; Goldstein et al., 2008). Thus, relational aggression
appears to be a relevant concern in both peer and romantic relationships among college
students.
The present study aimed to contribute to the relatively meager research on
relational aggression in the context of college students’ romantic relationships by
exploring the role of interpersonal jealousy. A number of personality and individual
difference factors have been shown to predict relationally aggressive behaviors in college
students’ peer relationships and intimate partnerships; however, the relationship between
romantic jealousy and relational aggression in college students’ dating relationships has
not been adequately investigated. It is hoped that learning more about this potential
relationship can eventually inform efforts to understand, prevent, and treat relational
aggression in this population.
Literature Review
Relational Aggression
Relational aggression refers to a set of behaviors intended to manipulate and/or
damage another’s relationship, reputation, or feelings of belonging or inclusion (Linder et
al., 2002; Prather et al., 2012). Although relational aggression is typically viewed as a
type of indirect aggression, it can be direct or indirect. Examples of indirect relational
aggression include gossip, spreading rumors, and subtle forms of manipulation; whereas,
more direct forms include overt social exclusion or threats to end a relationship if the
victim does not conform to the aggressor’s wishes (Czar, Dahlen, Bullock, & Nicholson,
2011). In the context of romantic relationships, relational aggression is associated with
psychosocial maladjustment, problem behavior, and lower levels of relationships quality
3
(Goldstein, 2011). Linder et al. (2002) found that both relational aggression and
victimization in romantic relationships were associated with negative relationship
features involving reduced trust and elevated jealousy.
Compared with overt forms of aggression (i.e., verbal and physical aggression),
far less is known about relational aggression. This is especially true among older
adolescents and early adults, as most of the relational aggression literature has focused on
the peer relationships of children and early adolescents (Goldstein & Tisak, 2004).
Goldstein (2011) agreed that relational aggression had been shown to be relatively
common among in youths’ relationship formation and further noted that when it occurred
in same-sex friendships, relational aggression was especially salient for young women,
and that compared to young men, young women were more bothered by relational
aggression and spent more time thinking about it and discussing it.
Among older adolescents and early adults, it is clear that relational aggression in
peer relationships is detrimental. A growing body of research has identified a number of
adverse correlates of relational aggression among college students, such as peer rejection
and insecure attachment, substance use, antisocial behaviors and psychopathic
personality traits, anxiety, and depression (Linder et al., 2002). Although it has received
less attention in the literature than peer relational aggression, romantic relational
aggression (i.e., relationally aggressive behaviors occurring in the context of intimate
partnerships) also appears problematic (Prather et al., 2012). Prather and colleagues
(2012) found that the acceptance of couple violence was positively related to the
perpetration of relational aggression in dating relationships, indicating that individuals
4
who hold less negative attitudes toward couple violence would be more likely to engage
in relational aggression in their intimate partnerships.
Although a number of variables have been identified that appear useful in
predicting relational aggression in both the peer and romantic relationships of college
students, the potential role of jealousy is unknown. Since some of the relationally
aggressive behaviors that occur in the context of romantic relationships seem to involve
attempts to provoke jealous reactions in one’s partner (e.g., flirting with someone else in
front of one’s partner, withdrawing one’s time and attention from one’s partner and
reallocating them elsewhere), it seems that students who tend to be jealous in their
romantic relationships may be more relationally aggressive.
Jealousy
At the broadest level, romantic jealousy refers to a complex set of emotions,
cognitions, and behaviors (Melamed, 1991; Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). The emotional
experience of jealousy is varied. For some individuals, the experience may be
characterized primarily by anger. For others, feelings of shame, rejection, or sadness
may dominate the picture (Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). The cognitive component of
jealousy often involves how one interprets the quality of one’s relationship and various
threats to it (e.g., the perception of real or imagined attraction between one’s partner and
a rival). As Pfeiffer and Wong (1989) noted, interpretations of threats are not necessarily
rational and include worries or suspicions without supporting evidence. The behavioral
component of jealousy refers primarily to how individuals cope when jealous. According
to Pfeiffer and Wong (1989), some of these behaviors are aimed at detecting threats (e.g.,
searching a partner’s belongings for evidence of infidelity) and others seek to protect the
5
relationship from perceived threat (e.g., showing up unannounced to interrupt a potential
encounter between one’s partner and a suspected rival). Some jealous behaviors (e.g.,
exerting excessive control over one’s partner) are often viewed as forms of emotional or
psychological abuse (Ellis et al., 2009) and may be a precursor to intimate partner
violence (Elphinstion, Feeney, Noller, Connor, and Fitzgerald, 2013). Nemeth, Bonomi,
Lee, and Ludwin, (2012) found that anxiety about sexual fidelity, a common feature of
jealousy, is used as a strategy by perpetrators to control victims. This suggests one way
in which jealousy via infidelity concerns may serve as both a trigger for the acute violent
episode and as a more persistent tactic for sustained abuse over time.
Jealousy is a common phenomenon in romantic relationships and appears to be
relevant in many cases of relationship dissatisfaction. Approximately one third of
couples under the age of 50 seeking counseling identified jealousy as a primary reason
for pursuing professional help (Worley & Sampson, 2014). Elphinstion and colleagues
(2013) found that the most common jealousy-evoking situation between romantic
partners involved one partner’s choice to give time and attention to someone who is not
their partner.
Jealousy has been found to be a factor behind many negative relationship
experiences, such as intimate partner violence, verbal and physical aggression, and
relational dissatisfaction and uncertainty (Elphinstion et al., 2013). Knobloch, Solomon,
and Cruz (2001) also found that the experience and expression of jealousy was associated
with a host of negative intrapersonal and relational outcomes, including relational
dissatisfaction. They also found that cognitive jealousy was closely tied to relational
uncertainty. Communication and jealousy have been found to be related not only to
6
relational satisfaction, but also to relationship stability and permanence, with
communication associated positively and jealousy associated negatively. Communicative
responses to jealousy are significantly more predictive of relational satisfaction than
jealousy experience alone (Andersen, Elvoy, Guerro, & Spitzberg, 1995).
The Present Study
The present study explored the possible relationship between jealousy and
romantic relational aggression in a college student sample. Research investigating these
issues is important, given the evidence that relational aggression is problematic among
college students and the increased salience of romantic relationships during this
developmental stage (Goldstein & Teran, 2008; Linder et al., 2002). With so little
evidence of a relationship between these constructs, the present study should be viewed
as an exploratory investigation designed to determine whether assessing individual
differences in jealousy is likely to have utility in understanding relational aggression. It
was predicted that jealousy would be positively related to romantic relational aggression.
That is, we expected that college students higher in jealousy would report higher levels of
romantic relational aggression. Although we had no basis for making specific predictions
with regard to the possible role of participant gender, we included this variable to
determine whether there were gender differences on the variables of interest. Finally,
although we were primarily interested in the cognitive dimension of jealousy (i.e., the
degree to which one worries about one’s partner’s romantic interest in others), we
selected a multidimensional measure of jealousy that also permitted is to assess the
emotional and behavioral aspects of jealousy.
7
Methods
Participants
Participants were 377 undergraduate student volunteers (64 men and 313 women)
ranging in age from 18 to 58 (Mdn age = 20) recruited through the Department of
Psychology’s subject pool. In terms of their racial/ethnic backgrounds, participants
identified themselves as African American (31%), Caucasian (62.9%), Hispanic/Latino
(1.3%), American Indian (.5%), Asian (1.3%), and other (2.9%). With regard to their
sexual orientation, most participants identified themselves as heterosexual or straight.
Freshmen were somewhat overrepresented with the numbers of sophomores, juniors, and
seniors roughly equivalent. Most of the sample did not belong to Greek organizations,
and slightly more than half approximately half lived off campus. Additional information
about the demographic characteristics of participants can be found in Table 1. Eligibility
to participate in the study required participants to be either involved in a current romantic
relationship (73.7% of the sample) or to have been involved in a romantic relationship at
some time during the previous 12 months (26.3% of the sample). Of those currently
involved in a relationship, most involved opposite-sex (i.e., male-female) relationships
(94.6%), followed by same-sex relationships involving two women (4%), and same-sex
relationships involving two men (1.4%). Many of these relationships were long term,
lasting more than 2 years (40.3%), and most were described as dating relationships
(71.9%). Additional information about the current relationships of participants currently
involved in romantic relationships and previous relationship of those not currently
involved in a romantic relationship can be found in Table 2 and Table 3, respectively.
8
Measures
Demographic Questionnaire. A brief demographic questionnaire was used to
collect information about participants’ gender, age, race, sexual orientation, relationship
status, and living arrangements. Most of this information was used to describe the
sample; however, the questions about age and relationship status were also used to make
sure that participants qualified to participate in the study.
Multidimensional Jealousy Scale (MJS; Pfeiffer & Wong, 1989). The 24-item
MJS was used to assess jealousy across three domains: cognitive, emotional, and
behavioral. All items were rated on a 1 to 7 scale; however, the scale anchors differ for
each of the domains. Items on the cognitive domain and behavioral domains are rated as
to their frequency of occurrence. Specifically, cognitive items are rated from 1 (“all the
time”) to 7 (“never”), and behavioral items are rated from 1 (“never”) to 7 (“all the
time”). Items on the emotional domain are rated by how pleased vs. upset each situation
would make the participant from 1 (“very pleased”) to 7 (“very upset”). Although the
associations of all three domains to romantic relational aggression were of interest, the
primary domain of interest for this study was the cognitive domain, as it provides the
most direct measure of the degree to which one worries about his or her partner’s interest
in a romantic rival as well as interest shown in one’s partner by a rival. Russell and
Harden (2005) reported the reliability coefficients of .82 for the cognitive scale, .90 for
the emotional scale, and .81 for the behavioral scale. The MJS has been used in a number
of studies, and comparisons with other measures of jealousy have supported its validity.
Self-Report of Aggression and Social Behavior Measure (SRASBM; Linder et
al., 2002; Morales & Crick, 1998). Romantic relational aggression was measured using
9
the 5-item Romantic Relational Aggression subscale from the 56-item SRASBM.
Respondents rated each item on a 7-point scale to indicate how true it is for them from 1
(“not at all true”) to 7 (“very true”). Czar and colleagues (2011) used this scale and
reported reliability coefficients of .72 to .76 range. Evidence of convergent validity has
been provided in the form of correlations with measures of romantic relationship quality
and several indices of psychological adjustment.
Procedure
Potential participants were recruited through the online research system used by
the Department of Psychology (i.e., Sona; https://usm.sona-systems.com/). After reading
a brief description of the study posted through Sona, potential participants who were
interested signed up for the study. After signing up, they received a URL directing them
to an online consent form and all survey measures hosted through Qualtrics. They were
informed about the possible risks and benefits of the study, reminded that participation
was voluntary and could be discontinued at any point during the study without penalty,
and provided with the primary researcher’s contact information. Students who wished to
participate provided electronic consent before being directed to the study questionnaires,
all of which were administered online through Qualtrics. To protect the anonymity of
participants and make sure that it was not possible to link them to their responses,
informed consent was obtained by using a yes/no item where potential participants must
select "yes" in order to see any study questionnaires instead of collecting names or
electronic signatures. The amount of time required to complete the study was
approximately 30 minutes. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board
10
(IRB) at the University of Southern Mississippi as part of a larger study on relational
aggression.
Results
Data Screening
The electronic data were downloaded from Qualtrics and saved as an SPSS data
file. Although this data file initially appeared to contain data from 472 respondents, 47
cases contained nothing but missing data and were deleted. This is fairly typical for
online survey studies conducted using the Department of Psychology’s subject pool and
probably reflects participants who accessed the survey and then decided not to complete
it at that point in time.
The data file was then examined for cases with excessive missing data on the
variables of interest. Three cases in which respondents omitted the entire Romantic
Relational Aggression subscale of the SRASBM were deleted. This is far less missing
data than is typically encountered in online survey research using this subject pool.
Next, two procedures recommended in the online survey research literature (e.g.,
Huang, Curran, Keeney, Poposki, & DeShon, 2012; Meade & Craig, 2012) were utilized
to identify and remove participants who responded carelessly. First, responses on two
directed response items (e.g., “Please answer ‘agree’ to this question”) that had been
blended into the online questionnaires were examined. Thirty-two participants failed one
or both of the directed response items and were removed from the data file. Second,
survey completion time was examined in order to identify participants who completed the
survey so quickly that it is unlikely that they could have read the instructions on each
questionnaire and attended to item content. This led to the removal of another seven
11
respondents. These numbers were somewhat lower than what is typical for online survey
research conducted with this subject pool, suggesting that participants in this study were
somewhat more likely to attend to the instructions and item content when answering.
Several analyses were then completed to check for the presence of univariate
outliers (i.e., cases with extreme scores on any variable of interest). Given the size of the
sample, a conservative criterion of z > 4 was used to identify univariate outliers. On this
basis, the data from five additional participants were omitted from, resulting in the final
sample size (N = 377) used in all analyses.
Preliminary Analyses
Alpha coefficients and descriptive statistics for all variables can be found in Table
4. The internal consistencies of the scales were adequate (i.e., αs > .70) to excellent,
suggesting that the scales used to measure each variable were assessing unitary
constructs. Participants obtained higher mean scores on MJS Emotional than on the MJS
Cognitive or Behavioral subscales, indicating that they were likely to report that they
would be upset across a number of situations in which their partner behaved in ways that
might raise concerns about their fidelity. As expected, the distributions of all variables
were skewed, which violates the assumption of normality required for parametric
statistics. The Romantic Relational Aggression subscale of the SRASBM and both the
Cognitive and Behavioral subscales of the MJS were positively skewed (i.e., most
participants obtained low scores on these variables, leading scores to cluster on the left of
the distributions); the Emotional subscale of the MJS was negatively skewed (i.e., most
participants obtained high scores on this variable, leading scores to cluster on the right of
the distribution).
12
Based on the desire to utilize parametric statistics, the distributions of these
variables were transformed to achieve normality. Specifically, logarithmic
transformations were applied to the Romantic Relational Aggression, MJS Cognitive, and
MJS Behavioral subscales. A reflected square-root transformation was applied to the
MJS Emotional subscale. These transformations normalized the distributions, and
transformed scores were used in subsequent analyses unless otherwise noted.
One-way (gender) Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) were computed to determine
whether women and men differed on any of the variables of interest (see Table 5). In
order for the means and standard deviations by gender to be comparable with those for
the full sample presented in Table 4, non-transformed scores were used in these analyses.
Given the degree of skewness previously noted, ANOVAs were computed using
bootstrapping methods to account for the non-normality of the data. There were a total of
1,000 random samples generated with 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals. There
were no statistically significant gender differences on any variable. Thus, women and
men in the present study did not differ on any of the MJS subscales or the Romantic RA
subscale of the SRASBM.
Primary Analyses
Interrelationships among variables were examined via bivariate correlations for
the full sample (see Table 6). We predicted that interpersonal jealousy, as measured by
the MJS, would be positively related to romantic relational aggression. This hypothesis
was supported, as scores on all three subscales of the MJS (i.e., cognitive, emotional, and
behavioral) were positively related to scores on the Romantic RA subscale of the
SRASBM. Participants who reported having more frequent jealous thoughts about their
13
romantic partner (MJS Cognitive), being more upset when faced with potential indicators
of their partner’s infidelity (MJS Emotional), and/or engaging in behaviors indicative of
jealousy (MJS Behavioral) reported being more likely to engage in relationally
aggressive behavior in the context of their romantic relationships.
Given that each of the three MJS subscales was related to the Romantic Relational
Aggression subscale of the SRASBM, a hierarchical multiple regression was computed in
which romantic relational aggression was regressed on respondent gender, age, and each
of the three MJS subscales. This type of analysis allows us to determine whether all three
subscales of the MJS were relevant to romantic relational aggression when considered
together and while taking respondent gender and age into account. Respondent gender
and age were entered in Step 1, and each of the three MJS subscales were entered
simultaneously on Step 2.
The full regression model was significant, R = .53, F (5, 376) = 28.82, p = .000.
The addition of the three MJS subscales on Step 2 resulted in a significant change in R
2
(see Table 7). Each of the three MJS subscales predicted scores on the Romantic
Relational Aggression subscale while taking respondent gender and age into account.
The relative contributions of the MJS Cognitive and Behavioral subscales were
comparable and larger than the contribution of the MJS Emotional subscale.
Discussion
This study was conducted in order to explore whether there was a relationship
between interpersonal jealousy and romantic relational aggression in a college student
sample. Despite the clear theoretical connection between these variables, the possible
role of jealousy among intimate partners had not been previously investigated in the
14
context of romantic relational aggression. The main findings of the study were that
interpersonal jealousy was positively related to romantic relational aggression and that no
significant gender differences were observed on either jealousy or romantic relational
aggression. While all three forms of jealousy assessed by the Multidimensional Jealousy
Scale (i.e., cognitive, emotional, and behavioral) were positively related to romantic
relational aggression while taking respondent gender into account, the cognitive and
behavioral dimensions explained more of the unique variance in romantic relational
aggression than the emotional dimension.
Our prediction that jealousy would be positively related to romantic relational
aggression in college students’ dating relationships was clearly supported. Although we
were most interested in the relationship of the cognitive component of jealousy to
romantic relational aggression, all three subscales of the MJS (i.e., cognitive, emotional,
and behavioral) were positive predictors of romantic relational aggression while
accounting for respondent gender. Thus, students who reported having more frequent
jealous thoughts about their romantic partner (MJS Cognitive), being more upset when
faced with potential indicators of their partner’s infidelity (MJS Emotional), and/or
engaging in behaviors indicative of jealousy (MJS Behavioral) were more likely to report
engaging in relationally aggressive behavior in their romantic relationships. This finding
is consistent with the work of Linder and colleagues (2002), which showed that
relationally aggressive behaviors in college students’ dating relationships were associated
with a number of negative relationship qualities. In addition, our findings were
consistent with those of Ellis and colleagues (2009), who found that victims of romantic
15
relational aggression generally reported insecure dating relationships, while perpetrators
reported distrust and jealousy in their relationships.
The present study did not find evidence of gender differences on jealousy or
romantic relational aggression. That is, male and female students in the present sample
did not differ on any of the MJS subscales or in terms of romantic relational aggression.
This is somewhat of a departure from Goldstein’s (2011) finding that relational
aggression in same-sex friendships was more salient for young women than young men;
however, it should be noted that the participants in the present study were older than
those in Goldstein’s study and that the present study focused on romantic rather than peer
relational aggression. The lack of gender differences observed in romantic relational
aggression is consistent with previous studies that found no gender differences in peer or
romantic relational aggression among college students (e.g., Czar et al., 2011; Prather et
al., 2012). Considered in this context, the present findings provide additional evidence
that the widely held belief that relational aggression is far more common among women
may be erroneous when it comes to emerging adults.
Limitations
There were a number of limitations of the present study that should be considered.
First, the use of a college student sample limits the degree to which the results can be
generalized to a non-college population of the same age range as well as to the broader
adult population. Second, women were overrepresented in the present sample (i.e.,
roughly 83% of the participants were women). While there are considerably more
women than men enrolled at the university from which the sample was collected, women
were still overrepresented in the present sample relative to their numbers across the
16
university. Third, this study relied on self-report measures which were highly face valid,
raising questions about issues of response bias or the possible lack of insight into one’s
own behaviors. Although it is not clear how one could reasonably assess constructs such
as cognitive jealousy without relying heavily on self-report measures, the study of
relational aggression would certainly benefit from supplementing self-report data with
other data collection methods (e.g., comparing self-report data with other-report data).
Finally, it is important to note that the research design utilized here was correlational and
that it does not permit the inference of causal relationships. While the present findings
are consistent with the possibility that jealousy leads to romantic relational aggression,
they are also consistent with the possibility that romantic relational aggression leads to
jealousy or that a third variable leads to both jealousy and romantic relational aggression.
Future Directions
The present findings suggest that the construct of interpersonal jealousy,
especially its cognitive and behavioral dimensions, is likely to be relevant in
understanding relational aggression in college students’ romantic relationships. In
addition to addressing the above limitations through the collection of data from more
diverse samples and the application of innovative methodologies that extend beyond self-
report data, future research should develop and evaluate more sophisticated models of the
relationship between jealousy, romantic relational aggression, and other relevant
variables. There is a growing body of evidence linking a variety of personality traits to
relational aggression among emerging adults. Perhaps jealousy should be considered
alongside some of the personality traits known to be relevant in relational aggression.
For example, might certain aspects of self-esteem (e.g., global self-esteem, contingent
17
self-esteem) moderate the relationship between jealousy and relational aggression? It
will also be important going forward to examine jealousy as a situational factor rather
than viewing it only as a stable trait. For example, experimental manipulations designed
to provoke jealousy among participants would not only help to assess the direction of the
relationship between jealousy and relational aggression but would also allow researchers
to determine whether jealousy is best conceptualized as a dispositional factor or as a
situational one. Another intriguing possibility involves the study of jealousy and
relational aggression in high-risk populations (e.g., perpetrators of intimate partner
violence). For example, a study of jealousy and romantic relational aggression conducted
with perpetrators of intimate partner violence could provide valuable information about
their role in overt aggression and violence.
In sum, the present study provides future researchers with a useful starting point
by demonstrating that interpersonal jealousy, as assessed with the Multidimensional
Jealousy Scale, is positively related to romantic relational aggression, as assessed with
the Romantic Relational Aggression subscale of the Self-Report of Aggression and Social
Behavior Measure, in a college student sample. It is hoped that this will lead to
additional work aimed at providing a more comprehensive understanding of the role of
jealousy in relational aggression.
18
References
Andersen, A. P., Elvoy, V. S., Guerro, K. L., & Spitzberg, H. B. (1995). Romantic
jealousy and relational satisfaction: A look at the impact of jealousy experience
and expression. Communication Reports, 8(2), 78-85. Retrieved from:
http://web.ebscohost.com.lynx.lib.usm.edu
Czar, A. K., Dahlen, R. E., Bullock, E. E., & Nicholson, C. B. (2011). Psychopathic
personality traits in relational aggression among young adults. Aggressive
Behavior, 37, 207214. Retrieved from:
http://web.ebscohost.com.lynx.lib.usm.edu/
Ellis, E. W., Crooks, V. C., & Wolfe, A. D. (2009). Relational aggression in peer and
dating relationships: Links to psychological and behavioral adjustment. Social
Development, 18(2), 254-269. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00468.x
Elphinstion, A. R., Feeney, A.J., Noller, P., Connor, P.J., & Fitzgerald, J. (2013).
Romantic jealousy and relationship satisfaction: The costs of rumination.
Western Journal of Communication, 77(3), 293-304. doi:
10.1080/10570314.2013.770161
Goldstein, S. (2011). Relational aggression in young adults’ friendships and romantic
relationships. Personal Relationships, 18, 645656. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-
6811.2010.01329.x
Goldstein, E. S. & Teran, C. D, & McFaul, A. (2008). Profiles and correlates of
relational aggression in young adults’ romantic relationships. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 37: 251-265. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9255-6
19
Goldstein, E. S., & Tisak, S. M. (2004). Adolescents’ outcome expectancies about
relational aggression within acquaintanceships, friendships, and dating
relationships. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 283-303.
doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.11.007
Huang, J. L., Curran, P. G., Keeney, J., Poposki, E. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2012).
Detecting and deterring insufficient effort responding to surveys. Journal of
Business and Psychology, 27(1), 99-114. doi: 10.1007/s10869-011-9231-8
Knobloch, K. L., Solomon, H. D., & Cruz, G. M. (2001). The role of relationships
developments and attachment in the experience of romantic jealousy. Personal
Relationships, 8, 205-224. Retrieved from:
http://web.ebscohost.com.lynx.lib.usm.edu
Linder, R. J., Crick, R. N., & Collins, A. W. (2002). Relational aggression and
victimization in young adults’ romantic relationships: Associations with
perceptions of parent, peer, and romantic relationship quality. Social
Development, 11, 1: 69-83. Retrieved from:
http://web.ebscohost.com.lynx.lib.usm.edu
Meade, A. W., & Craig, S. (2012). Identifying careless responses in survey data.
Psychological Methods, 17(3), 437-455. doi: 10.1037/a0028085
Melamed, T. (1991). Individual differences in romantic jealousy: The moderating effect
of relationship characteristics. European Journal of Social Psychology, 21, 455-
461. Retrieved from: http://web.ebscohost.com.lynx.lib.usm.edu
Morales, J. R., & Crick, N. R. (1998). Self-report measure of aggression and
victimization. Unpublished measure.
20
Nemeth, M. J. Bonomi, E. A. Lee, A. M. & Ludwin, M. J. (2012). Sexual infidelity as
trigger for intimate partner violence. Journal of Women’s Health, 21 (9) 934-949.
doi: 10.1089/jwh.2011.3328
Pfeiffer, S. M., & Wong, T. P. P. (1989). Multidimensional jealousy scale. Journal of
Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 181-196.
Prather, E., Dahlen, R. E., Nicholson, C. B., & Yowell, B. E. (2012). Research on
aggression in dating relationships: Relational aggression in college students’
dating relationships. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 21, 705-
720. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2012.693151
Russell, B. E. & Harton, C. H. (2005). The “other factors”: Using individual and
relationship characteristics to predict sexual and emotional jealousy. Current
Psychology: Developmental Learning Personality Social 24 (4), 242257.
Retrieved from: http://web.ebscohost.com.lynx.lib.usm.edu
Werner, N., & Crick, N. (1999). Relational aggression and social-psychological
adjustment in a college sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(4), 615
623. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.108.4.615
Worley, T. & Sampson, J. (2014). Exploring the associations between relational
uncertainty, jealousy about partner’s friendships, and jealousy expression in
dating relationships. Communication Studies, 65(4), 370-388. doi:
10.1080/10510974.2013.833529
21
Table 1
Demographic Characteristics of Participants (N = 377)
Characteristic
n
Gender
Male
64
Female
313
Race/Ethnicity
African American/Black
117
Caucasian/White
237
Hispanic/Latino
5
American Indian/Alaskan Native
2
Asian
5
Other
11
Year in College
Freshman
132
Sophomore
76
Junior
86
Senior
83
Member of Sorority or Fraternity
Yes
94
No
283
Live On or Off Campus
On Campus
175
Off Campus
202
Type of Residence
Dorm
156
Greek House
16
Apartment On Campus
5
Apartment Off Campus
113
With Parent(s)
29
House Off Campus
58
Sexual Orientation
Heterosexual or Straight
350
Gay or Lesbian
6
Bisexual
20
Other
1
22
Table 2
Information About the Relationships of Participants Currently in a Romantic
Relationship (N = 278)
Characteristic
n
%
Parties in the Relationship
Woman & Man
263
94.6
Man & Man
4
1.4
Woman & Woman
11
4.0
Length of Relationship
< 1 Month
6
2.2
1-3 Months
21
7.6
3-6 Months
27
9.7
6-9 Months
24
8.6
9-12 Months
20
7.2
1-2 Years
68
24.5
More Than 2 Years
112
40.3
Type of Relationship
Dating
200
71.9
Live Together
35
12.6
Engaged
14
5.0
Live Together and Engaged
11
4.0
Married
18
6.5
23
Table 3
Information About the Most Recent Relationship of Participants Not Currently in a
Romantic Relationship (N = 99)
Characteristic
n
%
Parties in the Relationship
Woman & Man
97
98.0
Man & Man
2
2.0
Woman & Woman
0
0
Length of Relationship
< 1 Month
6
6.1
1-3 Months
22
22.2
3-6 Months
18
18.2
6-9 Months
9
9.1
9-12 Months
8
8.1
1-2 Years
20
20.2
More Than 2 Years
16
16.2
Type of Relationship
Dating
93
93.9
Live Together
4
4.0
Engaged
1
1.0
Live Together and Engaged
1
1.0
Married
0
0
24
Table 4
Alpha Coefficients and Descriptive Statistics (N = 377)
Measure
α
M (SD)
Skewness
MJS
Cognitive
.95
20.74 (13.05)
1.02
Emotional
.81
43.10 (7.21)
-0.55
Behavioral
.84
17.62 (8.83)
1.15
SRASBM
Romantic RA
.72
10.55 (5.35)
1.23
Note. MJS = Multidimensional Jealously Scale; SRASBM = Self-Report of Aggression
and Social Behavior Measure; RA = relational aggression.
Table 5
Univariate Gender Comparisons (N = 377)
Men (n = 64)
Women (n = 313)
Measure
M (SD)
95% CI
M (SD)
F (1,376)
MJS
Cognitive
20.81 (12.86)
17.53-24.28
20.73 (13.10)
.00, ns
Emotional
42.47 (6.93)
40.68-44.16
43.22 (7.27)
.58, ns
Behavioral
16.53 (9.23)
14.43-18.83
17.84 (8.74)
1.17, ns
SRASBM
Romantic RA
10.09 (5.30)
8.87-11.53
10.65 (5.37)
.56, ns
Note. MJS = Multidimensional Jealously Scale; SRASBM = Self-Report of Aggression
and Social Behavior Measure; RA = relational aggression; CI = 95% bias-corrected
confidence interval using 1,000 bootstrap resamples of the data.
25
Table 6
Intercorrelations Among Variables (N = 377)
Measure
1
2
3
4
1. MJS Cognitive
-
2. MJS Emotional
.00
-
3. MJS Behavioral
.41**
.17*
-
4. SRASBM Romantic RA
.43**
.15*
.44**
-
Note. MJS = Multidimensional Jealously Scale; SRASBM = Self-Report of Aggression
and Social Behavior Measure; RA = relational aggression.
*p < .01. **p < .001.
Table 7
Hierarchical Regression Analysis Summary for Respondent Gender, Age, and Jealousy
Predicting Romantic Relational Aggression (N = 377)
Variable
B
SE B
β
R
2
ΔR
2
Step 1
.00
Gender
.03
.03
.05
Age
.00
.00
.03
Step 2
.28
.28**
MJS Cognitive
.24
.04
.31**
MJS Emotional
.02
.01
.10*
MJS Behavioral
.30
.05
.29**
Note. MJS = Multidimensional Jealously Scale.
*p < .05. **p < .001.
26
INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD
118 College Drive #5147 | Hattiesburg, MS 39406-0001
Phone: 601.266.5997 | Fax: 601.266.4377 | www.usm.edu/research/institutional.review.board
NOTICE OF COMMITTEE ACTION
The project has been reviewed by The University of Southern Mississippi Institutional
Review Board in accordance with Federal Drug Administration regulations (21 CFR 26,
111), Department of Health and Human Services (45 CFR Part 46), and university
guidelines to ensure adherence to the following criteria:
The risks to subjects are minimized.
The risks to subjects are reasonable in relation to the anticipated benefits.
The selection of subjects is equitable.
Informed consent is adequate and appropriately documented.
Where appropriate, the research plan makes adequate provisions for monitoring
the data collected to ensure the safety of the subjects.
Where appropriate, there are adequate provisions to protect the privacy of
subjects and to maintain the confidentiality of all data.
Appropriate additional safeguards have been included to protect vulnerable
subjects.
Any unanticipated, serious, or continuing problems encountered regarding risks
to subjects must be reported immediately, but not later than 10 days following the
event. This should be reported to the IRB Office via the “Adverse Effect Report
Form”.
If approved, the maximum period of approval is limited to twelve months.
Projects that exceed this period must submit an application for renewal or
continuation.
PROTOCOL NUMBER: 15021001
PROJECT TITLE: Romantic Relationships in College
PROJECT TYPE: New Project
RESEARCHER(S): Eric Dahlen, Ph.D.
COLLEGE/DIVISION: College of Education and Psychology
DEPARTMENT: Psychology
FUNDING AGENCY/SPONSOR: N/A
IRB COMMITTEE ACTION: Expedited Review Approval
PERIOD OF APPROVAL: 02/11/2015 to 02/10/2016
Lawrence A. Hosman, Ph.D.
Institutional Review Board